Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Emotional stimuli candidates for behavioural intervention in the prevention of early childhood caries: a pilot study

Abstract

Background

Oral diseases, such as early childhood caries (ECC), have a complex etiology with common, behaviour-related risk factors. Appropriately targeted behavioural intervention using effective tools can help to eliminate risk behaviour leading to ECC. The aim of this study was to ascertain which visual stimuli with a supporting text evoke the strongest emotional response in infants’ mothers and, therefore, are suitable candidates for inclusion in behavioural interventions within the prevention of ECC.

Methods

Thirty-nine mothers of one-year-old children who filled out an originally designed electronic questionnaire, containing 20 visual stimuli with accompanying texts related to dental caries (10/10 with positive/negative intended emotional response), were included in this cross-sectional study. The emotional impact of each stimulus in the mothers was evaluated using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) technique, which represents three emotional dimensions: valence, arousal, and dominance.

Results

Each of the stimuli was assessed by the mothers of infants based on its emotional impact. The real emotional response (evaluated according to the median of valence) was in line with the primarily intended response in 90% of cases (p < 0.05). The text with a warning evoked a greater emotional response (evaluated according to the median of arousal) in mothers than only the informative instruction (p < 0.05). The relationship between arousal and valence (r = − 0.99; p < 0.05) indicates that the more aversive stimuli raise higher arousal. The significant correlation between valence and dominance shows that the more positive the stimuli, the higher feeling of control over the evoked emotion the mothers have (r = 0.83; p < 0.05), and, on the contrary, the lowest control over emotion is correlated with higher arousal (r = − 0.85; p < 0.05). Generally, mothers rated themselves as in high control of their emotions over the individual stimuli.

Conclusions

This pilot study proved that negative pictorial and text warnings about the risks of developing caries had the potential to evoke strong emotional responses in the mothers of infants. We identified three visual stimuli that could be included in future extensive motivation material in an attempt to affect the preventive behaviour of mothers, and thus the oral health of their infants.

Peer Review reports

Background

Dental caries is a multifactorial disease caused mainly by the presence of microbial plaque on the teeth surface related to poor oral hygiene, inappropriate dietary habits, and low tooth surface exposure to fluoride [1]. Despite a number of preventive measures, dental caries remains one of the most widespread infectious diseases in the world. Early childhood caries (ECC) is defined as the presence of one or more decayed (non-cavitated or cavitated lesions [d1-d4]), missing (due to caries), or filled tooth surfaces in any primary tooth in a child under the age of six [2]. Severe ECC (s-ECC) is any sign of smooth-surface caries in a child younger than 3 years of age [2]. Based on the etiopathogenesis of ECC, it is possible to define non-modifiable (genetic and socioeconomic) and modifiable (dietary habits and oral hygiene) factors related to this oral disease. It has been suggested that one of the key factors in the etiology and subsequently the prevention of dental caries, is the behaviour of mothers or other caregivers [3, 4]. Despite satisfactory knowledge of the risk factors for dental caries, information alone is not sufficient to change behaviour and a special behavioural intervention is necessary [5].

Emotions are closely linked to the approach and avoidance motivation strategies which represent the fundamental motivational forces governing human behaviour. They can be viewed as “push” forces that predispose to act in a certain way [6]. Each motivated behaviour can be divided into two tendencies by activating two different brain systems: the tendency to approach a pleasant stimulus (reward system) and the tendency to repulse from an aversive stimulus (defence system) [7]. The general aim of behaviour could be defined as an effort of people to move closer to the desired emotional state. In other words, actions in reaction to emotions should lead to the achievement of a more ‘good for me’ or less ‘bad for me’ state [8]. In order to evoke the person’s motivation to behave in a certain way, the stimulus that is to provoke must be connected with the emotional reaction, in the sense of the basic assessment as to whether it is a good or bad state for the person.

Currently, most information on dental caries prevention is presented as a professional recommendation in a rather emotionally neutral textual form. The “evidence-based” recommendation produces positive emotions in health care providers, for others it is rather neutral and incomprehensible. Although verbal expressions of emotions and feelings in humans are very variable, factor-analytic studies of emotional language have concluded that each stimulus can be placed into a three-dimensional affective space which is defined by: emotional valence, emotional arousal and dominance [9]. These dimensions are closely connected with the motivation of behaviour. The coordinates of the stimulus (e.g., photograph, text, sound, etc.) in the affective space, expressed by valence, arousal and dominance, represent important information about its motivational power.

In this context, searching for effective approaches to motivate people to change risk behaviour or to create health-promoting behaviour, is a major challenge of preventive medicine.

Methods

The aim of this pilot study was to ascertain which visual stimuli with a supporting text evoke the strongest emotional response in infants’ mothers and, therefore, are suitable candidates for inclusion in behavioural interventions within the prevention of ECC.

The originally designed electronic questionnaire (Additional files 1 and 2) contained 20 stimuli, which represented the risk factors of dental caries in children or showed the appropriate behaviour of mothers in the care of their children’s dentition. The visual stimuli (10 with an intended positive response and 10 with an intended negative response), accompanied by verbal descriptions, warnings or information (instruction), were designed in line with the recommendations of the European Academy of Pediatric Dentistry [1]. The visual stimuli were chosen according to the experience of the clinicians from our out-patients clinic. The most serious conditions were included in aversive pictures. The positive picture set was composed of the most desired aims of prevention of dental caries (pictures 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 12, 14, 16, 18, and 20).

The emotional impact of these 20 individual stimuli on mothers was evaluated using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) method (Fig. 1) [10]. The SAM graphical scale is a tool for the assessment of the three basic emotional dimensions: valence, arousal, and dominance. The emotional valence (first line in Fig. 1) is a bipolar scale which describes a continuum between polarities that are extremely unpleasant (unhappy manikin on the left) and extremely pleasant (happy manikin on the right). The scale ranges from 1 to 9 (1 extremely unpleasant, 5 neutral, 9 extremely pleasant). The arousal scale (the second line in Fig. 1) represents a continuum which ranges from 1 (calm, relaxed manikin on the left) to 9 (extremely excited, aroused manikin on the right). The dominance scale (third line in Fig. 1) describes the level of subjectively referred sense of control over the emotion evoked by the stimulus. It ranges from 1 (loss of control, manikin on the left) to 9 (full control, manikin on the right). The subject is instructed to evaluate their immediate subjective feeling being evoked by the test material (texts and pictures in our study) across the three afore-mentioned dimensions.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Self-Assessment Manikin scale [18]

During the period from 04/2017 to 10/2017, 97 mothers of one-year-old children were addressed with a request to join this cross-sectional study. Ninety-seven infants were examined with a dental mirror and probe by one pediatric dentist at the Clinic of Stomatology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University and St. Anne’s Faculty Hospital. The d1mft index was calculated using dental caries (d1 level) as a cut-off point for the detection of decay. There were no exclusion criteria for mothers and infants. Only 39 mothers signed informed consent forms and agreed to participate in our study. The basic demographic characteristics (age of mothers, education of both parents), and their awareness of caries risk factors and dental caries prevention, i.e. suitable diet, drinking sweet beverages, significance of teeth cleaning/brushing, suitable teeth brushing techniques, observance of oral health of their children etc., were determined in all of them. Subsequently, after being instructed as to how to record the emotional perception evoked in them by a stimulus on the SAM scale, (see Additional file 1), the mothers were asked to fill out the electronic questionnaire. The link to the electronic questionnaire was sent to mothers’ e-mail addresses, the questionnaire was filled out online.

For statistical purposes, the scope of the SAM technique was converted to numbers ranging from 1 to 9. The software Statistica v. 13 (IBM Corporation, 2013) was used for the data analysis. The relationships between the 3 dimensions (valence, arousal, dominance) were computed using Spearman’s correlation coefficient (r). For the description of variables the mean and standard deviation (SD), median and interquartile range (IRQ), and minimum and maximum were used. Statistical significance of relationship between categorical variables was tested using Fisher exact test, statistical significance of differences in ordinal scores between groups was tested using Mann Whitney U test. The sample size was planned on the basis of expected width of 95% confidence interval for average score estimate with expected standard deviation of pseudo-continuous scoring scale 1. The acceptable width for the 95% confidence interval of average score estimate was set as 0.7; it corresponds to N = 35. The computation of the overall sample size was estimated using previously experienced response rate approximately 30–40% in similar studies [11,12,13]. The response rate to the questionnaires was estimated to be assumed to be approximately 40%, thus 97 mothers were addressed to reach 35–40 responses.

Results

In this pilot study, 39 mother-child pairs were included. The electronic questionnaire response rate was 40.2%. 35.9% of mothers were in the age group of 21–30 years and 64.1% were in the age group of 31–40 years. 51.3% mothers had higher (university) education. Only four mothers had lower than secondary school education. The mean age ± standard deviation (SD) of the infants was 11.2 ± 2.4 months (median age [interquartile range, minimum-maximum] 10.0 [10.0–12.0, 8.0–20.0] months) with 5.6 ± 3.8 (5.0 [3.0–7.0, 0–16.0]) primary teeth. The examination revealed beginning of caries in three children (d1mft) on all upper incisors, i.e. d1mft = 4, the other children were caries free.

Five mothers reported that they did not use tooth paste for cleaning their infants’ teeth, only two mothers served sweetened beverages to their children and none of them dipped a pacifier into something sweet before giving it to their child. Up to 28.2% of mothers cleaned a pacifier in their mouths and even 43.6% of them occasionally or regularly tasted a meal with the same spoon or licked the spoon while feeding the infant.

Each of the stimuli in the electronic questionnaire was defined based on its emotional impact on the infants’ mothers. Table 1 shows the statistical evaluations (median and interquartile range) of valence, arousal, and dominance produced by ranking the individual stimuli in the questionnaire. There was a highly statistically significant relationship between the expected and observed emotional result of the visual stimuli (10 negative and 10 positive vs. 8 negative and 12 positive, p < 0.05). The real emotional response (evaluated according to the median of valence) was in line with the primarily intended response in 90% of cases. Highly statistically significant difference in the median values of valence between groups defined according to the expected negative and positive emotional result was observed (10 intended negative emotional stimuli with median of valence 2 [IQR, 2–4] and 10 intended positive emotional stimuli with valence 9 [IQR, 8–9], p < 0.05). The text with a warning evoked a greater emotional response in mothers than only the informative instruction. The significant difference in median values of arousal between groups categorized according to the type of text was found (8 text warnings with median of arousal 6 [IQR, 5–7] and 12 text instructions with arousal 2 [IQR, 1–2], p < 0.05).

Table 1 Ranking of the stimuli in the questionnaire (valence, arousal, dominance)

Stimulus 5 was perceived as the most negative with the lowest valence of 1 and the highest arousal of 8, with 95% of the mothers giving the picture and the description a negative valence (valence less than 5) and 69.2% high arousal. Stimuli 3, 5, 7, 9, 13, 15, 17, and 19 exhibited low valence (median from all texts and pictures ≤5) and can be considered as negative. High arousal (median of all statements ≥5) was recorded only in stimuli 5, 7, 9, 13, 15, 17, and 19. Negative stimuli 5, 7, and 13 including text warnings were assessed by more than 75% of mothers with a valence ≤5 and at the same time with an arousal ≥5 (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Ranking of selected stimuli

Our results demonstrated that not all stimuli originally intended as negative were perceived by the infants’ mothers adversely. Negative stimuli 1 and 11 were evaluated by mothers with valence 7 and 8 (respectively), and thus as positive. However, stimuli 19 and 20 with an intended opposite response were evaluated by mothers as we had presumed, i.e. they evoked a rather neutral valence and a mean value of arousal (Fig. 2).

It is evident that the exposure of mothers to stimuli did not cause a significant loss of control over the evoked emotions (see Table 1). The median value of all responses was higher than 5 in all cases. For this reason, only the results relating to the remaining two subscales (i.e. valence and arousal) are further presented.

All positive images and descriptions generated positive valence and low arousal (Fig. 3). Stimuli 8, 10, 12, and 18 (with median of valence 9 and arousal 1) were evaluated as the most positive ones. The relationship between arousal and valence (r = − 0.99; p < 0.05) indicates that the more aversive stimuli raise higher arousal. The significant correlation between valence and dominance shows that the more positive the stimuli, the higher feeling of control over the evoked emotion the mothers have (r = 0.83; p < 0.05), and, on the contrary, the lowest control over emotion is correlated with higher arousal (r = − 0.85; p < 0.05). Generally, mothers rated themselves as in high control of their emotions over the individual stimuli.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Two-dimensional chart of mother’s emotional response to 10 positive and 10 negative stimuli

Discussion

Several studies have focused on behavioural interventions within prevention of ECC and other oral diseases. Behavioural interventions to reduce caries have been based on a variety of behaviour change theories and approaches − mainly the social cognitive theory and the related health belief model and the theory of planned behaviour, self-determination theory, and motivational interviewing [14, 15].

The preventive programs are primarily aimed at children at primary schools [16]. Programs trying to influence parental behaviour are rare [17]; they mostly affect the frequency of tooth cleaning but do not lead to a change of dietary habits in children [17]. We believe that intervention within prevention of ECC should be targeted at mothers as they play an important role in health behaviour of the whole family, especially infants.

As already mentioned, emotions are an important part of the motivation to change behaviour. Triggering positive or negative emotions associated with problematic behaviour is one of the fundamental processes of this change [18]. The most effective approaches in preventive medicine are those with minimal economic costs and maximum population impact [19]. Searching for stimuli that evoke strong emotions and address the relevant target groups is a key step of each effective preventive intervention.

This also was the aim of the submitted pilot study as the stimulus material for the intervention approach for the Czech population has not been formed and standardized yet. Stimuli 5, 7, and 13 in our questionnaire have the lowest valence and the highest potential to evoke a strong emotional response in the mothers of infants and thus may be used as aversive stimuli within behavioural interventions in the field of oral health. The highest arousal was evoked by a picture of the damaged teeth of a child and the statement “Lack of mother’s care for her child’s teeth causes tooth decay.” The high dominance associated with all stimuli (including stimulus 5) could be explained again by the fact that the experimental group probably consisted of responsible mothers, who visit the dentist regularly and have no reason to worry.

While the negative pictures with warnings were evaluated by mothers as we had presumed, both negative figures with educational texts were evaluated with an unexpectedly high valence. In stimulus 1, this can also be explained by the fact that the crossed-out positive figure does not evoke negative emotions. In stimulus 11, the figure shows a child being treated by a dentist, similarly as in stimuli 4, 9, 16, and 17. We suppose that the mothers of infants in our studied group perceived the dental treatment neutrally and thus these types of figures should be replaced by other images in following studies.

Nevertheless, stimuli 1 and 11 represent an interesting subgroup of pictures with supporting texts in terms of the emotional responses of mothers. The responses to them very likely depend on the behaviour of the mothers and thus constitute a continuum between positive and negative valence reactions. Mothers participating in this study cared for their children’s oral health and generally maintained preventive measures. Therefore, they appreciated this information as positive and might be willing to follow the recommendations. Negative responses could be expected from the mothers on the opposite side of the continuum whose behaviour is unhealthy and negative pictures with texts confront them with this fact.

In general, current prevention medicine uses only a limited repertoire of behaviour change techniques [20, 21]. It mostly focuses on providing information on the relationship between behaviour and health or fear appeal. For example, the global prevention campaign on tobacco products is based on aversive visual stimuli and text warnings. However, it has been repeatedly shown that education and intimidation are not enough [14] and that people are more motivated by moving towards positive emotions [22] than by avoiding negative situations. The approach often neglected by a contemporary biomedical model of medicine is based on the premise that rather than avoiding fear it is more natural for people to move towards some appreciated personal values and principles (e.g.,“to be a good mother”). A preventive campaign based on this principle should be the next focus of research into the prevention of tooth decay.

For this reason, positive stimuli were also included in our questionnaire, for example a picture of a mother who is cleaning her teeth with her child and the sentence: “Parents are the ultimate model for children”. This sentence was rated by mothers as one of the most positive, but with very low arousal. Finding that positive images are associated with lower arousal agrees with the results of other studies [23]. It is still unclear what role arousal plays in the positive motivation and what its optimal level for the most effective behavioural change should be.

The warning or information about the inappropriateness of kissing a child on the mouth (stimuli 19 or 20) due to the possible transfer of the cariogenic bacteria from the mother’s mouth into the child’s oral cavity evoked a rather neutral valence and a mean value of arousal, whereas the interquartile range is greatest at the valence of these stimuli. It probably reflects great ambivalence of mothers to this statement. The picture of mother kissing her child with information about a possible transmission of bacteria has the potential to be used as a positive stimulus in the intervention process.

The main limitation of this study is a low number of probands due to a lack of interest across a group of mothers of infants. The fact that the electronic questionnaire was filled out by the mothers online may have played a role in the low response rate. Mothers were not under pressure to participate. On the other hand, responders should be in emotional stable state when filling out the electronic questionnaire. The final number of the obtained questionnaires generated a sample size which was sufficient for the planned statistical estimates with a relevant confidence interval.

Another problem is the significant impact of selection, especially when mothers with higher motivation and generally better knowledge of risk factors of tooth decay (“conscious mothers”) were willing to be involved in the study. There is a high probability that mothers with risk behaviour were not interested in participating in the questionnaire survey. Further research will need to be done on a larger sample of the population, including mothers from risk groups.

Our constant hypothesis is that the stimulus material should firstly impress mothers with a negative warning, thus developing negative emotions in the form of concerns about their child’s health, and at the same time offer them a solution as to how to avoid the feared consequences of their behaviour. In the future follow-up study, we intend to create a portfolio of emotionally-charged stimuli (images with texts) and then verify their effectiveness in behavioural intervention. The stimuli material can be a beta version for a motivational interview or a smart phone application which we will test on the Czech population. We can draw inspiration from the studies in the population of the American Indians [24] or studies by Nolen et al. [25]. It will be necessary to determine the percentage of mothers who really have changed their behaviour on the basis of the given prevention method (reduced consumption of sweetened drinks, regular preventive examinations, etc.) and to verify whether the defensive mechanisms such as generalization or rationalization were not activated.

Conclusions

This pilot study proved that negative pictorial and text warnings about risks of developing caries had the potential to evoke strong emotional responses in the mothers of infants. We identified three visual stimuli which could be included in a future extensive motivation material in an attempt to affect the preventive behaviour of mothers, and thus the oral health of their infants. Future research in this area should focus on creating extensive stimulus material and its subsequent verification on a large population.

Abbreviations

d:

decay, part of d1mft decay/missing/filled teeth index

ECC:

early childhood caries

I:

information

IQR:

interquartile range

N:

negative

P:

positive

p:

p-value

r:

correlation coefficient

R:

emotional response of mothers to visual and text stimulus

S:

stimulus in the questionnaire

SAM:

Self-Assessment Manikin

SD:

standard deviation

s-ECC:

severe form of early childhood caries

T:

text stimulus

V:

intended visual stimulus

W:

warning

References

  1. The European Academy of Paediatric Dentistry. Guidelines on Prevention of Early Childhood Caries: an EAPD policy document. In: Policies and guidelines. The European Academy of Paediatric Dentistry. 2008. http://www.eapd.eu/uploads/1722F50D_file.pdf. Accessed 20 Nov 2018.

  2. American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry. Policy on early childhood caries (ECC): classifications, consequences, and preventive strategies 2017;39(6):59–61.

  3. Kukletova M, Izakovicova Holla L, Musilova K, Broukal Z, Kukla L. Relationship between gingivitis severity, caries experience and orthodontic anomalies in 13-15 year-old adolescents in Brno, Czech Republic. Community Dent Health. 2012;29(2):179–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Wilson AR, Mulvahill MJ, Tiwari T. The impact of maternal self-efficacy and oral health beliefs on early childhood caries in Latino children. From Public Health. 2017;5:228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kukletova M, Kuklova J. ECC and the possibilities of prevention [text in Czech, abstract in English]. Prakt Zubn Lek. 2004;52(5):150–6.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Frijda NH. Studies in emotion and social interaction. The emotions. New York: Cambridge University press, Paris: Maison de sciences de l'Homme; 1986.

  7. Schneirla TC. An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In: Jones MR, editor. Nebraska symposium on motivation. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1959. p. 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(2):85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum P. The measurement of meaning. 1st ed. Urbana: University of Illinois Press; 1957.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994;25(1):49–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ismail A, Nainar SM, Sohn W. Children’s first dental visit: attitudes and practices of US pediatricans and family physicians. Pediatr Dent. 2003;25(5):425–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Grembowski D, Spiekerman C, Milgrom P. Linking mother access to dental care and child oral health. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol. 2009;37(5):381–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Araia MH, Wilson J, Chakraborty P, Gall K, Honeywell C, Milburn J, Ramsay T, Potter BK. Factors associated with knowledge of and satisfaction with newborn screening education: a survey of mothers. Genet Med. 2012;14:963–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Albino J, Tiwari T. Preventing childhood caries: a review of recent behavioral research. J Dent Res. 2016;95(1):35–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Randall CL. On motivational interviewing for oral health promotion: state of the field and future directions. JDR Clin Trans Res. 2018;3(4):376–77.

  16. Cooper AM, O’Malley LA, Elison SN, Armstrong R, Burnside G, Adair P, et al. Primary school-based behavioural interventions for preventing caries. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013;5:CD009378. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/14651858.CD009378.pub2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. O'Malley L, Adair P, Burnside G, Robinson L, Coffey M, Pine C. An evaluation of a storybook targeting parental attitudes, intention, and self-efficacy to change their child’s oral health behavior. Health Psychol. 2017;36(2):152–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Prochaska JO, Norcross JC. Systems of psychotherapy: atranstheoretical analysis. 5th ed. Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole Pub; 2003.

  19. Velicer WF, Prochaska JO, Fava JL, Rossi JS, Redding CA, Laforge RG, et al. Using the transtheoretical model for population – based approaches to health promotion and disease prevention. Homeost Health Dis. 2000;40(5):174–95.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Abraham C, Michie S. Taxonomy of behavior change techniques used in interventions. Health Psychol. 2008;27(3):379–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Martino S. Oral health behavioral and social intervention research concepts and methods. J Public Health Dent. 2011;71Suppl 1:S2–S6.

  22. Seligman MEP. Authentic happiness: using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. 1st ed. New York: Free Press; 2002.

  23. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Fearfulness and affective evaluations of pictures. Motiv Emot. 1999;23(1):1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Batliner TS, Tiwari T, Henderson WG, Wilson AR, Gregorich SE, Fehringer KA, et al. Randomized trial of motivational interviewing to prevent early childhood caries in American Indian children. JDR Clin Trans Res. 2018;3(4):366–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Nolen SL, Giblin-Scanlon LJ, Boyd LD, Rainchuso L. Development and testing of a smartphone application prototype for oral health promotion. J Dent Hyg. 2018;92(2):6–14.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The study was supported by funds from the Faculty of Medicine MU to junior researcher Michaela Bartosova, project MUNI/A/1008/2017, by the Ministry of Health of the Czech Republic, grant no. 17-30439A, and by grant RECETOX Research Infrastructure supported by the Czech Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports (LM2015051) and the European Structural and Investment Funds, Operational Programme Research, Development, Education (CZ.02.1.01/0.0/0.0/16_013/0001761). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MK, MS and MB designed the study, prepared the questionnaire and drafted the manuscript. MB examined the infants and addressed mothers for participation in the study. MS, PBL, LIH and LD performed the statistical analysis and participated in the interpretation of data. PBL and LIH assisted with reviewing and revising the final manuscript. All authors approved the manuscript as submitted and take full responsibility for the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lydie Izakovicova Holla.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Committee for Ethics of the Faculty of Medicine Masaryk University Brno (1G/2017). We confirm that written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The images and photographs in Fig. 2 and Additional file 2 belong to the authors.

Consent for publication

Written consent for publication was obtained from all the infants’ caregivers in this manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Instructions for filling out the electronic questionnaire (PDF 124 kb)

Additional file 2:

Overview of the stimuli in the electronic questionnaire (S1-S20) (PDF 364 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bartosova, M., Svetlak, M., Kukletova, M. et al. Emotional stimuli candidates for behavioural intervention in the prevention of early childhood caries: a pilot study. BMC Oral Health 19, 33 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12903-019-0718-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12903-019-0718-4

Keywords